96
PILOTTI, BEYER, AND YASUNAMI
perimental trials per list type and did not include training
trials. Thus, their findings may reflect differences in how
separate groups of subjects initially adjust to the stimulus
suffix procedure. Of course, this argument does not apply
to the study by Parkinson and Perey (1980), who com-
pared the performance of 10 older adults in the stimulus
suffix paradigm with that of a large group of young adults
with different digit spans. In this experiment, subjects re-
called 120 lists (60 per list type) organized in blocks of
lobe deficits. In P. Rabbitt (Ed.), Methodology of frontal and execu-
tive function (pp. 39-59). Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press.
Manning K. S., & Greenhut-Wertz, J. (1990). Visual and auditory
modality and suffix effects in youngand elderly adults. Experimental
Aging Research, 16, 3-9.
Maylor, E. A., Vousden, J. I., & Brown, G. D. A. (1999). Adult age
differences in short-term memory for serial order: Data and model.
Psychology & Aging, 14, 572-594.
Parkin, A. J. (1997). Normal age-related memory loss and its relation
to frontal lobe dysfunction.In P. Rabbitt(Ed.), Methodologyof frontal
and executive function (pp. 177-190). Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press.
30. With the young adults’ data collapsed across digit Parkinson, S. R., & Perey, A. (1980). Aging, digit span, and the stim-
ulus suffix effect. Journal of Gerontology, 35, 736-742.
Penney, C. G. (1985). Elimination of the suffix effect on preterminal
items with unpredictablelist length:Evidence for a dual model of suf-
fix effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
span, age differences emerged in boththe terminal and the
preterminal suffix effects. In the present study, we also
found such differences in the blocked condition, suggest-
ing that the subjects in the Parkinson and Perey study fo-
cused attention on both the preterminal and the terminal
items of the recall lists. However, Parkinson and Perey did
not alert subjects to the effect of the suffix on recall prior
to the experimental session and did not examine perfor-
manceas a functionofpractice.Therefore, it is possiblethat,
in thisexperiment,the age differences observedon the ter-
minal component emerged only after the first two blocks
of trials—that is, after the subjects had the opportunity to
be exposed to both suffix and control lists.
& Cognition, 11, 229-247.
Shipley, W. C. (1940). A self-administering scale for measuring intellec-
tual impairment and deterioration. Journal of Psychology, 9, 371-377.
Taub, H. A. (1973). Memory span, practice, and aging.Journalof Geron-
tology, 28, 335-338.
Taub, H. A., & Long, M. K. (1972). The effects of practice on short-
term memory of young and old subjects. Journal of Gerontology, 27,
494-499.
Watkins, M. J., & Sechler,E. S. (1989). Adaptingto an irrelevant item
in an immediate recall task. Memory & Cognition, 17, 682-692.
Wingfield, A., Poon, L. W., Lombardi, L., & Lowe, D. (1985). Speed
of processing in normal aging: Effects of speech rate, linguisticstruc-
ture, and processing time. Journal of Gerontology, 40, 579-585.
Wiseman, T. A. (1998). Frequency, intrusions, and suffix effects in im-
mediate recall anddelayed recall as a functionofagingandAlzheimer’s
disease. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Graduate Center of the
City University of New York.
Zacks, R. T., & Hasher, L. (1994). Directed ignoring:Inhibitory regu-
lation of working memory. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), In-
hibitory processes in attention,memory, and language(pp. 241-264).
San Diego: Academic Press.
REFERENCES
Balota, D. A., & Engle, R. W. (1981). Structural and strategic factors
in the stimulus suffix effect. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Be-
havior, 20, 346-357.
Bloom, L. C., & Watkins, M. J. (1999). Two-component theory of the
suffix effect: Contrary findings. Journalof Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 25, 1452-1475.
Carlson, M. C., Hasher, L., Connelly, S. L., & Zacks, R. T. (1995).
Aging, distraction, and the benefits of predictable location. Psychol-
ogy & Aging, 10, 427-436.
Connelly,S. L., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1991). Age and reading:
The impact of distraction. Psychology & Aging, 6, 533-541.
Cowan, N. (1984). On long and short auditory stores. Psychological
Bulletin, 96, 341-370.
Crowder, R. G. (1971). The sound of vowels and consonants in imme-
diate memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 10,
587-596.
Crowder, R. G. (1978). Mechanisms of auditory backward masking in
the stimulus suffix effect. Psychological Review, 85, 502-524.
Crowder, R. G., & Morton, J. (1969). Precategorical acoustic storage
(PAS). Perception & Psychophysics, 5, 365-373.
Fabiani, M., & Friedman, D. (1997). Dissociations for temporal order
and recognition memory in aging. Neuropsychologia, 35, 129-141.
Greenberg, S. N., & Engle, R. W. (1983). Voice changes in the stim-
ulus suffix effect: Are the effects structural or strategic? Memory &
Cognition, 11, 551-556.
Greene, R. L. & Crowder, R. G. (1984). Modality and suffix effects
in the absence of auditory stimulation. Journal of Verbal Learning &
Verbal Behavior, 23, 371-382.
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension,
and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psy-
chologyof learning and motivation:Advances in research and theory
(Vol. 22, pp. 193-225). New York: Academic Press.
NOTE
1. Young adults’ recall scores were relatively high, raising the ques-
tion of whether high- and low-performing youngadults might have been
differentially affected by the presence of the suffix. To address this issue,
we organized young adults in two groups, high- and low-scoring sub-
jects, and assessed the effects of list type, practice, rate, and subgroupon
recall.
Between-subjects condition. The terminal suffix effect was larger for
the low-scoring subjects [F(1,60) = 14.76,MSe = 185.92],butsuch a dif-
ference didnot interact with either practice or rate. In contrast, the preter-
minal suffix effect illustrated a complex pattern of interactions involv-
ing practice, rate, and list type. Specifically, before practice, slowing the
presentation rate did notaffect the preterminal suffix effect in either low-
or high-scoring subjects (Fs < 1). After practice, slowing the presenta-
tion rate increased the preterminal suffix effect in low-scoring subjects
[i.e., it improved recall in the control lists more than in the suffix lists;
F(1,30) = 5.70, MSe = 86.39] and decreased it in high-scoring subjects
[F(1,30) = 13.37, MSe = 31.04].
Blocked condition (seven digits). The terminal and preterminal suffix
effects were larger forlow-scoringsubjects[F(1,48)= 18.04,MSe = 46.04,
and F(1,48) = 7.56, MSe = 25.69, respectively]. However, these differ-
ences did not interact with either rate or practice (Fs < 1).
(Manuscript received May 12, 2000;
Lowe, C., & Rabbitt, P. (1997). Cognitivemodels of ageing and frontal
revision accepted for publication October 11, 2001.)