Commentary/Gangestad & Simpson: Evolution and human mating
Western societies, the dominant paradigm for understanding and
controlling human sexuality derived from religion. Humans were
assumed to have a base nature, and only through the socialization
of religion could any semblance of order be maintained. In some
contemporary cultures, this paradigm continues to guide attempts
to control sexual behavior. In other cultures, religion-based con-
trol systems have been supplanted by secular legal, educational,
medical, and psychological systems, all of which are fed by the the-
ories and data of science.
From our perspective as professional applied psychologists, we
are concerned with how alternative scientific theories may be use-
ful in understanding sexual behavior, especially the kinds of be-
haviors that lead to negative consequences, and further, how ther-
apeutic efforts may be affected. Gangestad & Simpson’s (G&S’s)
account of how sexual strategies may have evolved is framed pri-
marily as a modification and extension of Buss and Schmidt’s
Putting people before parasites and places
Anne Campbell
Department of Psychology, Durham University, Science Laboratories,
Abstract: The strategic pluralism model depends upon pathogen preva-
lence and environmental hardship being independent. Evidence is pre-
sented that they are positively correlated. The rise in short-term mating
strategy in the United States is better explained by changes in the opera-
tional sex ratio than by increases in pathogen prevalence. Nonetheless, in
highlighting the advantages of a high-investment strategy to less attractive
males, Gangestad & Simpson’s model helps to clarify the dynamics of
frequency-dependent selection.
According to Gangestad & Simpson (G&S), variability in female
mating strategies derives from women’s different responses to
pathogen prevalence and harsh environmental conditions. The re-
lationship between the two is critical both theoretically and em-
pirically.
(
1993) Sexual Strategies Theory (SST) and both contrast with so-
cialization theories such as Eagly’s (Eagly 1987; Eagly & Wood
999), who proposes that culture is largely responsible for the dif-
1
ferences in how males and females act with regard to mating.
As therapists, we are often not as concerned with the actual
mechanisms supporting sexual behavior, but how patients and
therapists construe the causes of their behaviors. As Heider
Before addressing this, it is necessary to clarify the meaning of
harsh environments. Gangestad & Simpson define them tauto-
logically as “environments in which biparental care and stable
pair-bonds are needed and expected” (sect. 5.7.1). Effectively,
however, they treat them as characterised by resource scarcity
where “women could not provide for all the nutritional needs of
their offspring” (sect. 5.6.2). Such harsh environments also incite
competition for scarce resources (Clutton-Brock 1991, p. 257)
leading to high crime rates and other forms of social pathology
which exacerbate environmental challenges.
If parasites and harsh environment are negatively correlated
across geographical sites, then one female strategy would be uni-
versal since women everywhere would benefit from making the
same choice. If they are negatively correlated within a geograph-
ical site, for example, as a function of social class, then women of
the same class should all select the same strategy. If they are pos-
itively correlated (either between or within ecological niches), the
distinction between them evaporates and there is no possibility of
making differential predictions. Unless niche-independent ge-
netic differences in strategy choice are invoked, the viability of the
theory depends upon a near zero correlation between environ-
mental harshness and pathogen prevalence.
From an empirical viewpoint, there is much evidence that the
two are positively correlated and associated with poverty. Across
and within nations, indicators of parasite prevalence (infant mor-
tality rate, probability of dying before their fifth birthday, the
percentage of infants immunised, mortality from diarrhoeal and
respiratory infections, malaria, measles, tuberculosis) and harsh
environment (gross domestic product, death from accidental
causes, life expectancy, percentage of poor, national health ex-
penditure as a proportion of GNP, proportion of children whose
weight-for-age is acceptable by international standards) go hand
in hand (World Health Organisation 1999).
The rate of single-parent families has risen dramatically in
Western countries. In the United States between 1970 and 1990,
premarital births rose from 9% to 22% of births to white women
and from 42% to 70% to African-Americans (Weinraub & Grin-
glas 1995). According to G&S such a rise would be attributed to
higher pathogen prevalence yet this period has seen a rise in im-
munisation programs and in pre- and post-natal care. What has al-
tered in post-war years is the operational sex ratio (Guttentag &
Secord 1983; Pedersen 1991). Changes in attitudes to casual sex,
single parenthood, and divorce are strongly related to the relative
paucity of males, which puts them in a strong market position to
impose their preferred mating strategy. This situation is most
marked in the lowest social classes where the pool of eligible males
is further reduced by male unemployment, drug addiction, and
imprisonment (Campbell 1995). If this argument is correct, then
female mating strategies are frequency dependent and driven by
the availability and preferred strategy of males and females, rather
than by ecological factors in the environment.
(
1958) argued, we are all “naive” psychologists, and our informal
theories may serve to guide (and justify) much of our behavior. A
standard therapeutic assumption drawing from Eagly’s work
might be that sexual behavior is largely learned, responsive to
contemporary social structures, and thus malleable to a great de-
gree. Although the socialization that shapes the sexual behavior
of males and females is extremely powerful, therapists and clients
who view sexual behavior as largely socialized may nevertheless
view aberrant sexual behavior as potentially amenable to treat-
ment. On the other hand, as G&S emphasize, Buss and Schmitt’s
(
1993) SST can lead to the conclusion that “men may have
evolved over human evolutionary history a powerful desire for
sexual access to a large number of women” (Buss & Schmitt 1993,
p. 208). Although SST does acknowledge that both genders
should possess evolved methods for finding long-term mates,
“
men, more than women, are predicted to have evolved a greater
desire for casual sex” (Buss 1999, p. 162). Although Buss (1999)
takes care to note that a characteristic’s arising as a result of evo-
lutionary processes does not preclude that characteristic from
changing within an individual, a belief in SST would lead many
therapists and clients to view sexual desires and sexual behavior
as largely immutable, or at least highly resistant to modification.
If men have “evolved a greater desire for casual sex,” can society
(
or their female partners) blame them if they sometimes act upon
that desire? And are efforts to modify that desire action a waste
of time? G&S seem to offer a synthesis of the “strong nature” po-
sition of Buss and the “strong nurture” position of Eagly, in that
although evolved mechanisms are the primary focus, their mod-
ification of SST does not polarize males and females to a high de-
gree, and there is some recognition that contemporary environ-
mental factors may be responsible for the engagement of ST and
LT strategies in both sexes. As G&S acknowledge, there are many
questions unanswered by the present version of their theory, in-
cluding better specification of conditions that may call forth the
mating logic they propose for each gender. These explications
may eventually yield a theory that better informs the practice of
therapy related to sexual behavior.
For therapy to be effective, considering the naive theories held
by patient and therapist may be as important as knowing what ac-
tual mechanisms are responsible for the troubling behaviors in
question. Concordance of theory between patient and therapist
would seem to be crucial, and the prevailing views of society at
large are also important. Pessimistically enough, for some kinds of
sexual behaviors that concern us, none of this information may re-
sult in therapy being any more effective as a mechanism of pre-
vention or damage control than anything else has been through-
out recorded history.