Reproductive biology in Onitis species
101
one brood-mass (n = 2 O. belial, n = 3 O. anthracinus) and once (O. belial) we
found 4 eggs in a line at the end of the brood-mass. When a brood-mass held more
than one egg, the laying often (n = 13) seemed to have occurred within a relatively
long time span, as we found both eggs and larvae, or larvae in different develop-
mental stages, within the same nest. This was always found when females laid eggs
in cakes instead of brood-masses.
The mean number of eggs laid in a single nest was 5.7 1.8 in O. belial and
12.5 2.3 in O. anthracinus. For O. vanderkelleni, the only complete nest we were
able to observe contained 26 eggs, while in two other cases egg laying was inter-
rupted after 2 and 5 eggs had been produced.
We did not observe parental care, and adults always left the nests after the
completion of egg laying.
Larval behaviour. Hatching larvae grew up in almost spherical chambers dug
in the dung; the presence of several larvae in small spaces [as found in O. aygulus
(Fabricius 1781) by SKAIFE (1953), quoted by HALFFTER & EDMONDS (1982)], both in
brood-masses and in cakes, seems to confirm the ability of the larvae to repair
their chambers if they are damaged (KLEMPERER 1982). At the end of larval develop-
ment, pupation occurred within what was left of brood-masses or cakes, in cham-
bers deriving from those used by larvae.
DISCUSSION
As stressed in the introduction, contrary to what occurs in most genera of
coprophagous Scarabaeidae whose reproductive behaviour has been studied, nest-
ing behaviour in Onitis can include a quite wide range of modalities. The three
species we studied in the laboratory are underground nesters. Nesting begins short-
ly after the provision of fresh dung and follows what has been defined as the “tun-
neler” tactic (SATO 1998a, 1998b).
In O. vanderkelleni, nesting couples invest little in the building of elaborated
nests, as they seem to use mostly undifferentiated cakes. On the other hand, they
invest more resources in the laying of a relatively large number of eggs. This is not
new in Onitis and has been found, for instance, in the Afrotropical species O. aygulus
(cf. SKAIFE 1953). The use of the cake as a definitive location for the eggs is rare or
absent in O. belial and O. anthracinus. The preparation of independent brood-
masses, with eggs laid at regular spaces within them, or just one in a single brood-
mass, seems to reduce both spatial and trophic competitions. This tactic requires
greater energy investment in the building of the nest, but allows a reduction of the
number of eggs laid. O. belial does not seem to conform strictly to this behavioural
scheme, while O. anthracinus, O. westermanni Van Lansberge 1886 and other species
whose behaviour is known (HALFFTER & EDMONDS 1982) appear to adopt it quite
rigidly. The discovery of a nest of O. belial in which the brood-masses were located
inside a dung clump seems to corroborate the hypothesis of KLEMPERER (1982) that
brood-masses can, at least sometimes, be made by digging inside a large compact
structure (cake), previously brought under the soil surface, and subsequently filling
the holes in it with other dung brought from the surface. Moreover, our observations
on the behaviour of O. belial seem to confirm what we previously hypothesised, i.e.,
that the shaping of brood-masses follows the preparation of the cake and egg laying.