P244
HERBERT ET AL.
Address correspondence to Andrew M. Herbert, PhD, University of
North Texas, Department of Psychology, P.O. Box 311280, Denton, TX
76203-1280. E-mail: herberta@unt.edu
participants suggests they have more difficulty matching
elements across the symmetry axis. Such comparisons re-
quire an intact neural network spanning a few degrees of
visual angle, which correlates to a large area in the visual
cortex with interconnections between areas separated by
relatively large cortical distances.
References
Appelle, S. (1972). Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus
orientation: The “oblique effect” in man and animals. Psychological
Bulletin, 78, 266–278.
It is noteworthy that there was no difference in symmetry
detection for participants younger than age 60. In combina-
tion with evidence that low-level visual abilities are pre-
served in aging, the observed decline in symmetry detection
performance after age 60 suggests that observed difficulties
in higher level tasks may result from problems at all stages
of visual processing that accumulate with the number of
steps required to complete the visual computations (Habak
& Faubert, 2000; Hoyer & Plude, 1982; Walsh, 1988). A
diffuse neural loss occurring during nonpathological aging
would be expected to have a greater effect on higher level
tasks than on lower level tasks based on the increasing num-
bers of neurons and synapses involved in more complicated
processing (Spear, 1993; Weale, 1982). Habak and Faubert
(2000) showed that the perception of simple motion stimuli
(moving gratings) was similar in young and elderly groups,
but complex motion perception was worse in the older
group (where moving dot textures are presented). Complex
motion perception requires more processing steps than are
required for perceiving simple motion, and Habak and
Faubert argue that this is why the difference as a function of
age is primarily seen for certain types of moving stimuli. We
speculate that the decline in performance observed in the
present study reflects the fact that symmetry detection relies
on processes occurring after visual information arrives in
the primary visual cortex (Dakin & Herbert, 1998; Gurnsey
et al., 1998). This conclusion is speculative and requires fur-
ther study, because it has been demonstrated that older
adults use binocular cues just as well as younger subjects
when detecting masked Gabor patterns (Speranza, Mora-
glia, & Schneider, 1995). That finding is consistent with a
conclusion that more central processes related to binocular
vision must be intact in the older population. There were
some minor differences between the older and younger par-
ticipants in Speranza and colleagues’ study, which is consis-
tent with our finding that older participants could still detect
symmetry, just not as well as younger participants.
Barlow, H. B. (1980). The absolute efficiency of perceptual decisions.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B290, 71–
82.
Braitenberg, V. (1990). Reading the structure of brains. Network, 1, 1–11.
Crassini, B., Brown, B., & Bowman, K. (1988). Age-related changes in
contrast sensitivity in central and peripheral retina. Perception, 17,
315–332.
Dakin, S. C., & Herbert, A. M. (1998). The spatial region of integration for
visual symmetry detection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Lon-
don, B26, 659–664.
Dakin, S. C., & Watt, R. J. (1994). Detection of bilateral symmetry using
spatial filters. Spatial Vision, 8, 393–413.
Essock, E. A. (1980). The oblique effect of stimulus identification consid-
ered with respect to two classes of oblique effects. Perception, 9, 37–
46.
Gurnsey, R., Herbert, A. M., & Kenemy, J. (1998). Bilateral symmetry em-
bedded in noise is detected accurately only at fixation. Vision Research,
38, 3795–3803.
Habak, C., & Faubert, J. (2000). Larger effect of aging on the perception of
higher-order stimuli. Vision Research, 40, 943–950.
Herbert, A. M., & Humphrey, G. K. (1996). Bilateral symmetry detection:
Examining a “callosal” hypothesis. Perception, 2, 463–480.
Hoyer, W. J., & Plude, D. J. (1982). Aging and the allocation of attentional
resources in visual information-processing. In R. Sekuler, D. Kline, &
K. Dismukes, (Eds.), Aging and human visual function (pp. 245–263).
New York: A. R. Liss.
Julesz, B. (1971). Foundations of cyclopean perception. Chicago, IL: Uni-
versity of Chicago Press.
Lakshminarayanan, V., Aziz, S., & Enoch, J. M. (1992). Variation of the
hyperacuity gap function with age. Optometry and Vision Science, 69,
423–426.
Lakshminarayanan, V., & Enoch, J. M. (1995). Vernier acuity and aging.
International Ophthalmology, 19, 109–115.
Mach, E. (1906). The analysis of sensations and the relation of the physical
to the psychical. New York: Dover.
Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection theory: A user’s
guide. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Odom, J. V., Vasquez, R. J., Schwartz, T. L., & Lindbergh, J. V. (1989).
Adult Vernier thresholds do not increase with age; Vernier bias does.
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 30, 1004–1008.
Osorio, D. (1996). Symmetry detection by categorization of spatial phase,
a model. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B263, 105–110.
Owsley, C., Sekuler, R., & Boldt, C. (1981). Aging and low-contrast vision:
Face perception. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 21,
362–365.
Owsley, C., & Sloane, M. E. (1987). Contrast sensitivity, acuity, and the
perception of “real-world” targets. British Journal of Ophthalmology,
71, 791–796.
Owsley, C., & Sloane, M. E. (1990). Vision and aging. In F. Boller & R. J.
Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 4, pp. 229–249).
Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Palmer, S. E., & Hemenway, K. (1978). Orientation and symmetry: Effects
of multiple, rotational, and near symmetries. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 691–702.
Pashler, H. (1990). Coordinate frame for symmetry detection and object
recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
and Performance, 16, 150–163.
Rainville, S. J. M., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2000). The functional role of ori-
ented spatial filters in the perception of mirror symmetry—Psycho-
physics and modelling. Vision Research, 40, 2621–2644.
Rock, I., & Leaman, R. (1963). An experimental analysis of visual symme-
try. Acta Psychologica, 21, 171–183.
In conclusion, the salience of symmetry as a function of
orientation does not change, because this is most likely a di-
rect result of early visual processing, where different orien-
tations are treated differentially by cortical neurons (Ap-
pelle, 1972; Essock, 1980). The differences in symmetry
detection across age groups observed in the present study
are consistent with normal, but less effective, operation of
whatever neural circuitry is involved in symmetry detection.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by Medical Research Council of Canada
Grant MT-14777 to Jocelyn Faubert. We thank the observers and staff at Sir
Mortimer B. Davis Jewish General Hospital for their time and patience. Pre-
liminary results were presented at the 1999 annual meeting of the Associa-
tion for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Ft. Lauderdale, FL. Many
thanks to Dr. Bert Hayslip and three anonymous reviewers for their com-
ments on the manuscript. The ANCOVA was run in response to comments
from the reviewers.
Rubin, G. S., West, S. K., Munoz, B., Bandeen-Roche, K., Zeger, S.,
Schein, O., et al. (1997). A comprehensive assessment of visual impair-