PASSIVE VISITORS OR INDEPENDENT EXPLORERS
943
Teachers operating in something of a theoretical vacuum turned to psychologists and
to the theory and practice of behaviourism for guidance and inspiration. . .. Objectives
which could not be verified behaviourally were to be avoided. . . [Teachers] were told
that by shaping the antecedents (getting the conditions right), and controlling the
consequences (rewards and sanctions), they would be able to modify behaviour and
pupils would learn. (Byers 1994: 78-9)
The behaviourist methods were used first to modify difficult behaviour, and then
to teach desired behaviour. Some remarkable results were achieved, at least in the
et al.
teaching of specific skills (see for example Sebba
1993, Collis and Lacey
1996). However, from the early 1980s misgivings were increasingly expressed
about whether these methods could promote real understanding (McConkey
1981) or whether they encouraged passivity and dependence (Byers 1994,
Griffiths 1994). Recent years have seen strong arguments for more interactive
approaches and greater independence. However, the behaviourist model, if rarely
used now in its original form, still remains influential in Special Education.
The Warnock Report (Department of Education and Science, 1978) repre-
sented a shift away from a deficit classification towards the notion of a continuum
of need for all young people. It described a national population of children with the
basic right to an education, some of whom (estimated at 1 in 5) would at some time
during their education have Special Educational Needs to varying degrees.
‘a brave attempt to move away from categorisation
Tilstone (1991: 14) describes it as
and the consequent stigma’
, while noting that the new definitions were criticized by
some as vague and imprecise.
With the introduction of the National Curriculum for England and Wales
(NC) in 1988 some special educators feared that their pupils might once again
be sidelined (see, for example, Tilstone 1991) and have fought for the NC to be
viewed as ‘an entitlement’ for all children. Others (e.g. Locke 1996), while sup-
porting this view, have stressed the need to ensure that sufficient time and atten-
tion is given to the key social, physical, linguistic and cognitive skills that all
children need before they can access the NC. Locke (1996) argued that there is
little point in teaching a child how to fill a jug of water and calling it ‘science’
simply for the sake of ticking off a NC target. On the other hand suitably adapted
science activities might have much to offer children with SLD in terms of increas-
ing their knowledge of the world around them and also their desire to explore it. So
now seemed to be the right time to ask, not whether children with SLD are
entitled to learn science, but what it had to offer them.
Research at VISTA and other science centres
The short history of the science centre movement from the time when
Exploratorium opened in 1968 to the present day has been packed with variety
and controversy as to purpose (see Rennie and McClafferty 1996, for an excellent
exploring
for which there are written
review). At the heart of the controversy lies the dichotomy between
learning
,
which includes play and curiosity, and factual
instructions and explanations in addition to the prescribed activity. Other features
such as enjoyment, learning to use the equipment and reacting with others, have
also featured in the design of most centres and in the research into student
outcomes. Our purpose in this short review of design and intention is to focus