M324
KENNY ET AL.
perception, general health and mental health, declined over
12 months in both the placebo and treatment groups. Others,
similarly, have found no improvement in health perception
following testosterone or dihydrotestosterone replacement
in older men (22,23,32). The decline in health perception
scores may be due to the decline in health perception re-
ported with increasing age (31), though this is unlikely
given the relatively short duration of our study. More likely,
testosterone replacement may be associated with improved
health perception only in states of severe deficits as seen in
studies of younger, hypogonadal men. Further studies are
required to understand whether testosterone replacement is
associated with better health perception in this population.
Another explanation for the lack of effect of testosterone
therapy on self-perceived health may be due to the delivery
system. Both the testosterone and placebo patches were as-
sociated with skin irritation (40% in placebo group and 77%
in testosterone group) and itching. Itching predicted self-
perceived bodily pain at the 12-month survey, suggesting
that the rash and itching induced by the placebo and testos-
terone patches may be responsible for the overall lack of
improvement in health perception and for the deterioration
in self-perceived general and mental health in our study
sample. There has been another report of decline in quality-
of-life scores in a study involving allergic symptom induc-
tion (33). Other studies of transdermal testosterone or dihy-
drotestosterone delivery have also failed to demonstrate
change in health perception (22,32), but one study of 13
older, hypogonadal men receiving intramuscular testoster-
one reported a general increase in sense of well-being (34)
while another study of eugonadal men did not (23). Again,
further studies of testosterone supplementation using vari-
ous delivery systems are needed to determine whether there
are associated changes in health perception and whether
they are dependent on the mode of delivery.
Bioavailable testosterone levels at 12 months predicted
some aspects of health perception, including self-perceived
role limitations due to physical problems and self-perceived
vitality, as well as the physical component composite score.
This suggests that testosterone levels in older men may pos-
itively influence health perception associated with per-
ceived physical function. Snyder and colleagues also re-
ported improved self-perceived physical functioning in men
receiving testosterone compared with placebo (22). Testos-
terone has been implicated in the process of sarcopenia and
in the associated loss of muscle mass and strength (35), al-
though we did not find that strength predicted self-
perceived physical function scores. Further studies to
understand the mechanism by which testosterone predicts
physical function, other than by strength, are required.
The study has several limitations. The sample size is
small with a large number of men discontinuing prior to
study completion. This may have biased the results to only
the most robust individuals answering questions. While
baseline measures of changes in cognitive function were
only compared in the men who completed the study, we
may have missed an effect in less cognitively robust men.
Similar effects may have been missed in relation to health
perception.
available testosterone levels does not impair and may im-
prove cognitive function. Treatment did not improve health
perception, but this may have been due to the side effects of
skin irritation suggested by similar reactions in both the tes-
tosterone and placebo groups. Further studies of testoster-
one supplementation will be required to fully address the
role of testosterone in cognition and health perception in
older men with testosterone decline.
Acknowledgments
This work has been supported by the Patrick and Catherine Donaghue
Research Foundation, General Clinical Research Center (MO1-RR06192),
Claude Pepper OAIC (5P60-AG13631), and Dr. Kenny has been supported
with fellowships from the Brookdale Foundation and the Paul Beeson Fac-
ulty Scholar Program. In addition, we thank Pamela Fall and Christine
Abreu for assistance in running the biochemical assays and Julie Fenster for
assistance with data management. Testosterone and placebo patches were
provided by SmithKline Beecham at no charge to the study.
Address correspondence to Anne Kenny, MD, Center on Aging, MC-
5215, University of Connecticut Health Center, Farmington, CT 06030-
5215. Email: kenny@nso1.uchc.edu
References
1. Korenman SG, Morley JE, Mooradian AD, et al. Secondary hypogo-
nadism in older men: its relation to impotence. J Clin Endocrinol
Metab. 1990;71:963–969.
2. Khosla S, Melton LJ, Atkinson EJ, O’Fallon WM. Relationship of se-
rum sex steroid levels to longitudinal changes in bone density in young
versus elderly men. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2001;86:3555–3561.
3. Morley JE, Kaiser FE, Perry HM, et al. Longitudinal changes in tes-
tosterone, luteinizing hormone, and follicle-stimulating hormone in
healthy older men. Metabolism. 1997;46:410–413.
4. Flood JF, Morley PM, Morley JE. Age-related changes in learning,
memory, and lipofuscin as a function of the percentage of SAMP8
genes. Physiol Behav. 1995;58(4):819–822.
5. Gouras GK, Xu H, Gross RS, et al. Testosterone reduces neuronal se-
cretion of Alzheimer’s beta-amyloid peptides. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA. 2000;97(3):1202–1205.
6. Alexander GM, Swerdloff RS, Wang C, et al. Androgen-behavior cor-
relations in hypogonadal men and eugonadal men. II. Cognitive
abilities. Horm Behav. 1998;33(2):85–94.
7. Vogel W, Broverman DM, Klaiber EL, Abraham G, Cone FL. Effects
of testosterone infusions upon EEGs of normal male adults. Electroen-
cephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1971;31(4):400–403.
8. Kertzman C, Robinson DL, Sherins RJ, Schwankhaus JD, McClurkin
JW. Abnormalities in visual spatial attention in men with mirror
movements associated with isolated hypogonadotropic hypogonadism.
Neurology. 1990;40:1057–1063.
9. Cappa SF, Guariglia C, Papagno C, et al. Patterns of lateralization and
performance levels for verbal and spatial tasks in congenital androgen
deficiency. Behav Brain Res. 1988;31(2):177–183.
10. Christiansen K, Knussmann R. Sex hormones and cognitive function-
ing in men. Neuropsychobiology. 1987;18(1):27–36.
11. Gouchie C, Kimura D. The relationship between testosterone levels
and cognitive ability patterns. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1991;
16(4):323–334.
12. Barrett-Connor E, Goodman-Gruen D. Cognitive function and endog-
enous sex hormones in older women. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1999;47(11):
1289–1293.
13. Morley JE, Kaiser F, Raum WJ, et al. Potentially predictive and ma-
nipulable blood serum correlates of aging in the healthy human male:
progressive decreases in bioavailable testosterone, dehydroepiandros-
terone sulfate, and the ratio of insulin-like growth factor 1 to growth
hormone. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1997;94:7537–7542.
14. Janowsky J, Oviatt K, Orwoll E. Testosterone influences spatial cogni-
tion in older men. Behav Neurosci. 1994;108(2):325–332.
15. Janowsky J, Chavez B. Sex steroids modify working memory. J Cog-
nitive Neuroscience. 2000;12(3):407–414.
Transdermal testosterone treatment in men with low bio-
16. Cherrier MM, Asthana S, Plymate S, et al. Testosterone supplementa-