RACE AND FEELINGS OF CONTROL
S123
Another limitation involves drawing conclusions about
the effects of age on control using cross-sectional data.
Two issues raise concern in this respect. First, the direction
of causality between the predictors and control is unknown.
For example, with only one wave of data, the degree to
which increasing religion affects feelings of control cannot
be distinguished from the degree to which declines in con-
trol may cause changes in religiosity. In addition, with only
one wave of data, we cannot be sure if the observed pat-
terns in control are associated with age, period, or cohort
effects.
perspective. We hope that this research sparks further inter-
est in examining the significance of race, as well as other
social structural factors such as gender and socioeconomic
status, in the relationship between age and control.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by two grants from the National Institute on
Aging (“Well-Being Among the Aged: Personal Control and Self-Esteem,”
RO1 AG09221, Principal Investigator Neal Krause; and “Public Health and
Aging Training Program,” T32 AG 00134).
Address correspondence to Benjamin Shaw, Department of Health Be-
havior and Health Education, School of Public Health, University of
Michigan, 1420 Washington Heights, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-2029. E-mail:
Finally, there are limitations in some of the study mea-
sures. For instance, greater insight into the relationship be-
tween age and control may be achieved by using better
measures of religion and health. In addition, the measure
of social support used in this study requires some explana-
tion. As described in Table 1, levels of social support from
various sources (e.g., spouse, child, friends, and relatives)
were assessed separately. Therefore, because not all re-
spondents reported having relationships with people from
each source, we constructed a measure that reflects sup-
port from the person most likely to be closest to the re-
spondent (see Krause, Goldenhar, Liang, Jay, & Maeda,
1993, for a similar approach). Thus, for respondents who
were married or living with a partner, the measure reflects
support from this partner; for those without a significant
other but who had children, the measure reflects support
from their children; and for those who were without both a
significant other and children, the measure reflects support
from friends and relatives. This measure is based on an
implicit hierarchy reflecting the differential importance of
various sources of support. This means that if a significant
other is present, then it is assumed that the assistance he or
she provides matters most. If a respondent does not have a
significant other, but has children, it is assumed that sup-
port from a child is more important than support from the
remaining source assessed in this study (i.e., friends and
relatives). This measurement strategy, which differentially
weights support scores in favor of close family members,
is based on socioemotional selectivity theory (Carstensen,
1992). According to this perspective, as people age, emo-
tional support from family members becomes increasingly
important.
References
Allison, P. D. (1977). Testing for interaction in multiple regression. Ameri-
can Journal of Sociology, 83, 144–153.
Blustein, J., & Weitzman, B. C. (1995). Access to hospitals with high-tech-
nology cardiac services: How is race important? American Journal of
Public Health, 85, 345–351.
Caplan, G. (1981). Mastery of stress: Psychosocial aspects. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 413–420.
Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: Sup-
port for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 7,
331–338.
George, L. K., Landerman, R., Blazer, D. G., & Anthony, J. C. (1991).
Cognitive impairment. In L. N. Robins & D. A. Regier (Eds.), Psychi-
atric disorders in America: The Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study
(pp. 291–327). New York: Free Press.
Holden, C., Burkhauser, R., & Myers, D. (1986). Income transitions at
older stages of life: The dynamics of poverty. The Gerontologist, 26,
292–297.
House, J. S. (1997). Americans’ Changing Lives: Wave 1, 1986 (ICPSR
version) [electronic data file]. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan,
Survey Research Center [producer]. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university
Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor].
Krause, N. (1991). Stress and isolation from close ties in later life. Journal
of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 46, S183–S194.
Krause, N., Goldenhar, L., Liang, J., Jay, G., & Maeda, D. (1993). Stress
and exercise among the Japanese elderly. Social Science and Medicine,
36, 1429–1441.
Krause, N., & Tran, T. V. (1989). Stress and religious involvement among
older blacks. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 44, S4–S13.
Lachman, M. E., & Weaver, S. L. (1998). The sense of control as a moder-
ator of social class differences in health and well-being. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 763–773.
Livingston, I. L. (1994). Handbook of Black American health: The mosaic
of conditions, issues, policies, and proposals. Westport, CT: Green-
wood Press.
The measure of control used in our study could also be
improved. All three indicators available to us were keyed in
the direction of externality (i.e., less control). In addition,
the three items ask about feelings of personal control con-
nected to only negative outcomes (e.g., bad things and
death). However, more sophisticated instruments for mea-
suring control include items keyed towards both external
and internal locus of control, as well as both positive and
negative outcomes (Mirowsky & Ross, 1991). Nevertheless,
the fact that the pattern of decline in feelings of control
found in our data set is similar to those found in previous re-
search by other investigators suggests that our measure may
capture at least part of the same conceptual domain assessed
by more complete measures of control.
Maxwell, N. L. (1994). The effect on Black-White wage differences of dif-
ferences in the quality and quantity of education. Industrial and Labor
Relations Review, 47, 249–264.
Mendes de Leon, C. F., Seeman, T. E., Baker, D. I., Richardson, E. D., &
Tinetti, M. E. (1996). Self-efficacy, physical decline, and change in
functioning in community-living elders: A prospective study. Journal
of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 51B, S183–S190.
Mirowsky, J. (1995). Age and the sense of control. Social Psychology
Quarterly, 58, 31–43.
Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (1991). Eliminating defense and agreement
bias from measures of the sense of control: A 2 ϫ 2 index. Social Psy-
chology Quarterly, 54, 127–145.
Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (1998). Education, personal control, lifestyle and
health: A human capital hypothesis. Research on Aging, 20, 415–449.
Mirowsky, J., Ross, C. E., & Van Willigan, M. (1996). Instrumentalism in
the land of opportunity: Socioeconomic causes and emotional conse-
quences. Social Psychology Quarterly, 59, 322–337.
Mutran, E. (1985). Intergenerational family support among Blacks and
Whites: Response to culture or socioeconomic differences. Journal of
Gerontology, 40, 382–389.
Although there are limitations in the work we have done,
perhaps the greatest contribution of our research lies in our
effort to cast the study of race and control in a life course