2
26 j political science quarterly
people tend to interpret new information in the light of what they already be-
lieve, they are likely to be slow to change their views, and those most committed
4
to their beliefs will have the most difculty revising them. This suggests that
decision makers may not always be responsive to changes in the nature or level
of threat. Yet, as Reagan’s behavior demonstrates, they do sometimes over-
come their cognitive limitations to make fairly accurate assessments of threat.
What enables these decision makers to reevaluate threat successfully while oth-
ers remain prisoners of their predispositions?
The question is particularly intriguing in view of the numerous criticisms
that have been leveled at Reagan’s cognitive abilities. According to one ob-
server, for example, he was intellectually shallow and inconsistent, supercially
5
attached to different and not necessarily compatible beliefs. Moreover, David
Stockman gives us a picture of the President as both ignorant of the complexi-
6
ties of policy and profoundly muddled, while others note his lack of analytical
ability, lack of curiosity, and legendary dislike of detail, which, combined with
7
his ignorance of many issues, put him at the mercy of his advisers. Richard
Neustadt has remarked on Reagan’s unfortunate habit of “combining igno-
rance and insistence” (incuriosity about details together with deep commitment
8
to his convictions), which provided fertile soil for such ascos as the Iran-contra
9
scandal. Clearly, Reagan’s success in perceiving and responding to the changes
in Soviet policy, especially when many others did not, needs to be explained.
4
See Robert Jervis, Perception and Misperception in International Politics (Princeton: Princeton
University Press, 1976), chap. 7; Yaacov Vertzberger, The World in Their Minds (Stanford, CA: Stan-
ford University Press, 1990), 113–27; and Deborah Larson, Anatomy of Mistrust (Ithaca, NY: Cornell
University Press), 32–34.
5
Keith L. Shimko, Images and Arms Control: Perceptions of the Soviet Union in the Reagan Admin-
istration (Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1991), 236–37, 239–41, 246–47.
6
Richard Neustadt has collected a number of the references that convey Stockman’s view. Richard
E. Neustadt, “Presidents, Politics, and Analysis” (Brewster C. Denney Lecture Series, Institute of Pub-
lic Management, Graduate School of Public Affairs, University of Washington, 13 May 1986), 17. Oth-
ers who note Reagan’s ignorance and lack of curiosity in matters of policy are Shimko, Images, 245–46;
Lou Cannon, Reagan (New York: G. Putnam’s Sons, 1982), 372–73; Lou Cannon, President Reagan
(
New York: Simon and Shuster, 1991), 130; Alexander L. George and Juliette L. George, Presidential
Personality and Performance (Boulder, Colorado: Westview, 1988), 224; Richard E. Neustadt, Presi-
dential Power and the Modern Presidents (New York: Free Press, 1990), 270, 276; Michael Mandelbaum
and Strobe Talbott, Reagan and Gorbachev (New York: Vintage Books, 1987), 128–29.
7
George Shultz, Turmoil and Triumph (New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons, 1993), 1133; Martin
Anderson, Revolution (New York: Harcourt Brace, 1988), 289–91; Robert C. McFarlane, Special Trust
(
New York: Cadell and Davies, 1994), 106; Larry Speakes, Speaking Out (New York: Charles Scrib-
ner’s Sons, 1988), 67, 304; Cannon, Reagan, 375; Fred I. Greenstein, The Presidential Difference (New
York: Free Press, 2000), 146, 149; Larry Berman, “Looking Back at the Reagan Presidency” in Larry
Berman, ed., Looking Back on the Reagan Presidency (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press,
1
990), 5; Cannon, President Reagan, 55, 181–82, 304. Reagan’s ignorance of nuclear matters is particu-
larly striking, 291–92, 305.
8
Neustadt, Presidential Power, 270, 276, 280; see also Gary Wills, Reagan’s America (New York:
Penguin, 1988), 286, 380.
9
Neustadt, Presidential Power, 270–71, 287, 290.