P254
WETHERELL ET AL.
The sample was composed of racially and culturally ho-
ure Identification) than on working memory was not related
to state anxiety.
mogeneous Swedish twins; findings may differ in a more di-
verse sample. Twins may differ from single-birth individu-
als in ways that affect cognition or anxiety in later life,
although evidence suggests that registry-based twin samples
are representative, and findings from twin samples are com-
monly considered generalizable to a broader population.
Previous investigations have used twin samples such as the
National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Twin Study and
the Australian Twin Registry for longitudinal analyses,
treating participants as genetically unrelated individuals
The pattern of results further suggests that cognitive per-
formance is more sensitive to state anxiety than to neuroti-
cism, a proxy for trait anxiety. This would also be consistent
with processing efficiency theory’s emphasis on verbal pro-
cesses such as worry interfering with cognitive perfor-
mance. These processes are a state phenomenon, although
they may occur more frequently in individuals with high
trait anxiety.
Finally, this study provides no support for neuroticism as
a risk factor for cognitive decline. Although tasks that typi-
cally show age-related decline may be affected by anxiety,
general anxiety proneness does not appear to be related to
accelerated rates of decline. Of course, neuroticism is not
synonymous with trait anxiety. Future research should re-
visit the relationship between anxiety and cognitive decline
using a better measure of trait anxiety than neuroticism.
(
Dunne, Martin, Pangan, & Heath, 1997; Swan et al., 1998).
In this study, we used random effects models to allow for
the analysis of dependent observations, in effect controlling
for correlations between twin partners, to test hypotheses
about the relationship between anxiety and cognitive perfor-
mance across individuals. A related study could use these
models to explore individual differences by comparing pre-
dicted values to actual performance, but this was not the fo-
cus of the present investigation. A further limitation is that
approximately half of the sample were separated from their
twin partner in early life. This unusual feature may limit
generalizability, although age and education level, on which
differences were found between twins reared apart and
those reared together, were included as covariates in these
analyses.
Both memory measures in this battery tested visual, as
opposed to verbal, memory. Tests of nonverbal memory
have typically been overlooked in studies of anxiety in older
samples, an important reason for using them here. However,
because verbal memory tasks draw on resources from both
the phonological loop and the central executive of working
memory, they may be more vulnerable to the effects of anx-
iety than visual memory tests are.
Acknowledgments
The Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging is supported by National
Institute on Aging Grants AG-04563 and AG-10175, the MacArthur Foun-
dation Research Network on Successful Aging, and the Swedish Council
for Social Research. Support for the dementia evaluations comes from Na-
tional Institute on Aging Grant AG-08724. Julie Loebach Wetherell was
supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant NRSA MH-11972.
Julie Loebach Wetherell is now with the Department of Psychiatry, Uni-
versity of California, San Diego and the VA San Diego Healthcare System.
Address correspondence to Julie Loebach Wetherell, Department of
Psychiatry, University of California at San Diego, VA San Diego Health-
care System (116A-1), 3350 La Jolla Village Drive, San Diego, CA 92161.
E-mail: jwetherell@ucsd.edu
References
Baddeley, A. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. Boston: Allyn
and Bacon.
The main advantage of this large longitudinal study is the
ability to test hypotheses about the relationship between
anxiety and normal age-related cognitive decline, which has
apparently not been reported previously. The study included
a comprehensive test battery, including visuospatial and
visual-memory measures that have not been as frequently in-
vestigated among older adults as verbal memory or atten-
tional tasks in relation to anxiety. The sample is population-
based, eliminating the potential for bias from sources such
as memory clinics or senior centers. Furthermore, longitudi-
nal follow-up of the sample allowed us to limit analyses to
those participants who were cognitively intact, rather than in
the early stages of dementia.
Campbell, R. T. (1999). Why this issue? Research on Aging, 21, 131–143.
Cavanaugh, J. C., & Murphy, N. Z. (1986). Personality and metamemory
correlates of memory performance in younger and older adults. Educa-
tional Gerontology, 12, 385–394.
Cederlöf, R., Friberg, L., & Lundman, T. (1977). The interactions of smok-
ing, environment and heredity and their implications for disease etiol-
ogy. Acta Medica Scandinavia(Suppl. 612), 1–128.
Cederlöf, R., & Lorich, U. (1978). The Swedish Twin Registry. In W. E.
Nance, G. Allen, & P. Parisi (Eds.), Twin research: Part C. Biology and
epidemiology (pp. 189–195). New York: Alan R. Liss.
Cockburn, J., & Smith, P. T. (1994). Anxiety and errors of prospective
memory among elderly people. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 273–
2
82.
Cohen, D., Eisdorfer, C., Vitaliano, P. P., & Bloom, V. (1980). The relation-
ship of age, anxiety, and serum immunoglobulins with crystallized and
fluid intelligence. Biological Psychiatry, 15, 699–709.
Costa, P. T., Fozard, J. L., McCrae, R. R., & Bossé, R. (1976). Relations of
age and personality dimensions to cognitive ability factors. Journal of
Gerontology, 31, 663–669.
Davidson, H. A., Dixon, R. A., & Hultsch, D. F. (1991). Memory anxiety
and memory performance in adulthood. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
Conclusion
Overall, results provide some support for Eysenck’s pro-
cessing efficiency theory. State anxiety did result in poorer
performance, at least for men, on a task that draws heavily
on the phonological loop of working memory (Digit Span).
Likewise, performance on tasks involving the central execu-
tive, such as Analogies, Block Design, Names and Faces,
Thurstone’s Picture Memory, and—for men—Card Rota-
tions, was poorer for those reporting higher levels of state
anxiety. Performance on tasks that draw more on retrieval of
knowledge from long-term storage (Information) and per-
ceptual speed and pattern matching (Symbol Digit and Fig-
5
, 423–434.
DeFries, J. C., Plomin, R., Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1981). Par-
ent–offspring resemblance for cognitive abilities in the Colorado Adop-
tion Project: Biological, adoptive, and control parents and one-year-old
children. Intelligence, 5, 245–277.
Deptula, D., Singh, R., & Pomara, N. (1993). Aging, emotional states, and
memory. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 429–434.
Dunne, M. P., Martin, N. G., Pangan, T., & Heath, A. C. (1997). Personality
and change in the frequency of religious observance. Personality and
Individual Differences, 23, 527–530.