S106
KRAUSE ET AL.
1420 Washington Heights, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-2029. E-mail: nkrause@
umich.edu
needed on facets of religion that are relatively unique to
Asian cultures. For example, research on ancestor worship
was reviewed earlier (Klass, 1996), but we did not measure
this unique dimension of religion directly. Because ancestor
worship is a core component of religion in Japan, it is impor-
tant to devise specific survey measures of it and see whether
ancestor worship is related to health and well-being.
In the process of devising plans for further research, it is
important to keep the limitations of our study in mind.
Three shortcomings are discussed briefly below.
The first limitation in our work has to do with the mea-
surement of religious coping. Three indicators were used in
the present study to assess this construct, but as the work of
Pargament (1997) revealed, religious coping is a vast con-
ceptual domain that contains a large number of dimensions
and a wide range of specific attitudes and behaviors. Clearly,
more extensive measures of religious coping are needed for
further research in Japan.
Second, our measure of hypertension is based on respon-
dent self-reports. This raises obvious questions regarding
self-report bias. Although we presented data earlier that
suggest that self-reports may be reliable, researchers would
benefit from using other ways to assess hypertension, in-
cluding physician diagnoses, medical record checks, or di-
rect measurement blood pressure during the interview.
Third, as reported earlier, the reliability of the private re-
ligious practice measure used in this study is somewhat low
(.572). Although there is not complete consensus in the lit-
erature, some investigators have recommended that internal
consistency estimates should be at least .700 (Nunnally,
1978). However, the issue may not be as straightforward as
it initially appears. Our measure of private religious prac-
tices consists of only three indicators. This is important be-
cause it is well-known that coefficient alpha is sensitive to
the number of items in a scale. More specifically, it is
harder to attain estimates of the size recommended by Nun-
nally (1978) when there are only a few measures in an index
(Cortina, 1993). Even so, those wishing to conduct further
research on private religious practices would benefit from
using more reliable measures.
References
Benson, H. (1996). Timeless healing: The power and biology of belief.
New York: Scribner.
Brannigan, M. (1998). On asking the right questions: Personal death vs.
brain death in Japan. Death Studies, 22, 157–169.
Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory
and applications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 98–104.
Doi, T. (1973). The anatomy of dependence. Tokyo: Kodansha International.
Eliade, M. (1978). A history of religious ideas, Volume 1: From the Stone
Age to the Eleusinian mysteries (W. Trask, Trans.). Chicago: Univer-
sity of Chicago Press.
Ellison, C. G., & Levin, J. S. (1998). The religion-health connection: Evi-
dence, theory and future directions. Health Education & Behavior, 25,
700–720.
Gerin, W., Pickering, T. G., Glynn, L., Christenfeld, N., Schwarz, A., Carroll,
D., & Davidson, K. (2000). An historical context of behavioral models of
hypertension. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 48, 369–377.
Graig, L. A. (1999). Made in Japan: Universal health insurance. In L. A.
Graig (Ed.), Health of nations (pp. 91–120). Washington, DC: CQ
Press.
Health Services Bureau, Ministry of Health and Welfare. (1997). The na-
tional nutrition survey. Tokyo: Author.
Hood, R. W., Spilka, B., Hunsberger, B., & Gorsuch, R. (1996). The psy-
chology of religion. New York: Guilford.
Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (1989). Applied logistic regression. New
York: Wiley.
Jackson, C., Jatulis, D. E., & Fortman, S. P. (1992). The Behavioral Risk
Factor Survey and the Stanford Five-City Project Survey: A compari-
son of cardiovascular risk behavior estimates. American Journal of
Public Health, 82, 412–416.
Kasulis, T. P. (1990). Intimacy: A general orientation in Japanese religious
values. Philosophy East and West, 40, 434–449.
Klass, D. (1996). Ancestor worship in Japan: Dependence and the resolu-
tion of grief. Omega, 33, 279–302.
Koenig, H. G. (1999). The healing power of faith: Science explores medi-
cine’s last great frontier. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Koenig, H. G., McCullough, M. E., & Larson, D. B. (2001). The handbook
of religion and health. New York: Oxford University Press.
Krause, N. (1993). Measuring religiosity in late life. Research on Aging,
15, 170–197.
Krause, N. (1998). Neighborhood deterioration, religious coping, and
changes in health during late life. The Gerontologist, 38, 653–664.
Krause, N., Liang, J., Jain, A., & Sugisawa, H. (1998). Gender differences
in health among older adults. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics,
26, 141–159.
The death of a loved one is a truly universal event that is
experienced by virtually everyone around the world. More-
over, the odds of losing a significant other through death in-
crease as people grow older. Religion is also a universal
phenomenon in the sense that it is found in virtually every
major culture in the world. Consequently, it makes sense to
explore the interface between religion and the loss of a
loved one in cross-cultural settings. Doing so provides a ex-
citing forum for exploring how older people around the
world differ and are alike. We hope the findings reported in
this study encourage others to pursue this integral facet of
aging around the globe.
Krause, N., & Tran, T. V. (1989). Stress and religious involvement among
older blacks. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 44, S4–S13.
Levin, J. S. (2001). God, faith, and health: Exploring the spirituality-heal-
ing connection. New York: Wiley.
Levin, J. S., & Markides, K. S. (1985). Religion and health in Mexican
Americans. Journal of Religion and Health, 24, 60–69.
Levin, J. S., & Vanderpool, H. Y. (1989). Is religion therapeutically signif-
icant for hypertension? Social Science and Medicine, 29, 69–78.
Liang, J., Borawski-Clark, E., Liu, X., & Sugisawa, H. (1996). Transitions
in cognitive status among the aged in Japan. Social Science and Medi-
cine, 43, 325–337.
Lu, F. H., Tang, S. J., Wu, J. S., Yang, Y. C., & Chang, C. J. (2000). Hy-
pertension in elderly persons: Its prevalence and associated cardiovascular
risk factors in Taiwan City, Southern Taiwan. Journal of Gerontology:
Medical Sciences, 55A, M463–M468.
Marrone, R. (1999). Dying, mourning, and spirituality: A psychological
perspective. Death Studies, 23, 495–519.
Marsella, A. J. (1987). The measurement of depressive experience and dis-
order in different cultures. In A. J. Marsella, R. M. Hirschfeld, & M. M.
Katz (Eds.), The measurement of depression (pp. 376–397). New York:
Guilford.
Mattlin, J. A., Wethington, E., & Kessler, R. C. (1990). Situational deter-
minants of coping and coping effectiveness. Journal of Health and
Social Behavior, 31, 103–122.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by National Institute on Aging Grants R37
AG15124 (Jersey Liang, Principal Investigator), RO1 AG14749 (Neal
Krause, Principal Investigator), and T32 AG00134 (Public Health and Ag-
ing Program).
Address correspondence to Neal Krause, Department of Health Behavior
and Health Education, School of Public Health, University of Michigan,