AGE AND OCULAR MICROTREMOR ACTIVITY
M389
Table 2. Mean Values for 7 Parameters of Ocular Microtremor
Activity in Subjects Younger and Older Than 60 Years of Age
sory input (21). Studies by Shimada and colleagues (22)
looked at age-related changes in brain size in the SAM-R/1
mouse model. They found age-related atrophy only in a re-
stricted part of the cerebral cortex, mainly in the parietal re-
gion. Neural activity from the inferior parietal cortex is
known to impinge on the oculomotor nuclei (23). It may be
that a loss of cells in this area (or decrease in function) with
age could therefore reduce neural activity.
Interestingly, elderly subjects show a decreased fre-
quency of nystagmus as stimulated through the vestibular
system (9). Thus, physiological changes are not confined to
the sensory input level alone. Histological changes in extra-
ocular muscle with age (4) may account for a changing re-
sponse to neuronal input in elderly persons. The disturbance
of orderly fiber alignment and direction of the muscles
themselves would tend to reduce the frequency of move-
ment for any given input. Furthermore, increases in muscle
collagen content would tend to decrease the compliance of
the globe, altering resonance, and could also be responsible
for a reduction in OMT frequency, if the neuromechanical
model of OMT is accepted (18).
ꢂ60 Years
ꢁ60 Years
Parameter
of Age
of Age
t Value
p Value
Frequency, Hz
Baseline, %
Frequency Baseline, Hz
Number of Bursts
Frequency of Bursts, Hz
Duration of Bursts, ms
80.5 ꢀ 4.7
52.3 ꢀ 11.6
85.3 ꢀ 6.8
8 ꢀ 1.4
75.3 ꢀ 4.2
59.1 ꢀ 6.7
86.8 ꢀ 5.5
52.4 ꢀ 11
94.4 ꢀ 10.6
8.2 ꢀ 1.4
80.5 ꢀ 4.1
58.2 ꢀ 7.9
69 ꢀ 28.4
4.7
.0001
.99
.0001
.7
.0001
.64
.76
.012
3.64
.376
4.9
.44
.31
Duration of Baseline, ms 71.3 ꢀ 27.9
Subjects Younger and Older Than 60 Years of Age
Table 2 gives the mean value for each parameter for each
group. There is a highly significant drop in the overall fre-
quency, the frequency content of the baseline, and the fre-
quency content of the bursts in subjects older than 60 years
of age (p ꢁ .0001). There is no significant change in the
other parameters.
In latter years it has been recognized that recording OMT
may be of value in clinical situations, particularly as a
method of assessing brain-stem function (11). Studies have
shown that OMT activity is reduced in comatose patients
and could have prognostic value (14,15). More recently, our
group has shown that OMT activity is absent in subjects
who are clinically diagnosed as being brain-stem dead (24).
Studies have also shown that OMT activity is affected by
anesthetic agents and propose that monitoring OMT may
have a role in assessing the depth of anesthesia (11). In rela-
tion to the clinical application of OMT, these results are im-
portant. They suggest that different values of normality
should operate for subjects older than 60 years of age, at
least for frequency parameters. They also suggest that age
matching of subjects in controlled trials of OMT is essential
if valid conclusions are to be drawn from study results.
DISCUSSION
These results indicate a tendency for two of the three fre-
quency parameters of OMT (i.e., the overall frequency and
the frequency content of the bursts) to fall with advancing
age. Although the strength of the correlation for overall fre-
quency is small (r ꢅ ꢄ0.36), it remains significant (p ꢁ
.
002). The negative correlation between the frequency con-
tent of the burst and age is stronger (r ꢅ ꢄ0.534). In sub-
jects older than 60 years of age there is a significant fall in
all three frequency parameters of OMT activity (i.e., the
overall frequency, the frequency content of the baseline,
and the frequency content of the bursts). In each case the
difference is small, about 5.3 Hz for overall frequency, 9.1
Hz for baseline frequency, and 5.2 Hz for frequency content
of bursts. However, all these differences are significant.
However, a previous study by Coakley (17) compared
OMT in 26 young adults and 15 subjects older than 70 years
of age. He could find no significant change in OMT fre-
quency associated with age. This could be explained by the
small number of subjects studied. The analysis of the mi-
crotremor record in this study was based on velocity rather
than on displacement waveforms, as in our study. Velocity
against time analysis will enhance the contribution of
higher-frequency components to the overall measured fre-
quency.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by the Health Research Board, Ireland.
Address correspondence to Stana Bojanic, The Department of Neuro-
surgery, The Radcliffe Infirmary, Woodstock Road, Oxford OX2 1HE,
United Kingdom. E-mail: Stana.Bojanic@excite.co.uk
References
1. Birren JD, Woods AM, Williams MV. The behavioural slowing with
age. In: Poon LW, ed. Ageing in the 1980’s: Psychological Issues.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1980:293–308.
A significant change in frequency of either bursts or the
baseline will have a concurrent effect on overall frequency,
as we have seen. However, the other parameters studied are
also affected by age, although not significantly. This would
suggest that the overall pattern of OMT activity is not af-
fected significantly by age but that the firing frequency of
the extraocular motor neurones that lead to bursts are. These
findings are consistent with other noted changes in the ag-
ing ocular motor system. Increases in saccadic eye move-
ment latency are well documented (3,8). Changes in the sac-
cadic system are presumed to be due to age-related changes
in the ability to process sensory input. In the case of OMT,
however, the frequency of activity is not dependent on sen-
2
. Corso JF. Sensory processes and age effects in normal adults. J Geron-
tol. 1971;26:90–105.
3. Whitaker LA, Shoptaugh CF, Haywood KM. Effect of age on horizon-
tal eye movement latency. Am J Optom Physiol Optics. 1986;63:152–
1
55.
4
. Miller JE. Ageing changes in extra-ocular muscle. In: Lennerstrand G,
Baach-y-Rita P, eds. Basic Mechanisms of Ocular Motility and Their
Clinical Implications. Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press; 1974:47.
5. Chamberlain W. Restriction in upward gaze with advancing age. Am J
Opthalmol. 1971;71:341–346.
6
. Abel LA, Troost BT, Dell’Osso LF. The effects of age on normal sac-
cadic characteristics and their variability. Vision Res. 1983;23:33–37.
. Haywood KM. Eye movement pattern and accuracy during perceptual
motor performance in young and old adults. Exp Aging Res. 1982;8:
153–157.
7