MENTAL SIMULATION
375
people who imagine lifting a great deal of weight should the weights? Was there someone spotting you? Was there a sweaty
provide higher weight estimates than do people who sim- smell in the gym? Was it warm in the gym? Were you breathing heav-
ily after returning the bar to the bench? In addition to these questions,
ulate lifting less weight.
the participants also rated the vividness of their mental simulation
We conductedExperiment2C to determinethe extentto
experience on a scale ranging from 1 to 10 (1, extremely poor; 10,
extremely good).
The participants in the 5- and 10-simulation conditions then com-
which people use the amount of simulated weight as a ref-
erence point for their estimates. By this account, a mis-
2
attributionof familiarity does not influence performance pleted a 16-item survey that asked them to provide some basic demo-
estimates. Rather, peoplesimply use the amountof weight graphic information (e.g., age, gender), as well as some health re-
lated information (Do you smoke? Do you exercise on a regular basis?
in the simulation exercise to anchor their estimates. Par-
Compared with the average student, do you think that you are less
ticipants who simulate lifting 200 pounds may start at
healthy or more healthy? How many times have you visited the doc-
2
00 pounds and then shift their estimates downward to a
tor this year?). The critical survey item, which was the seventh ques-
tion on the survey, asked the participants to estimate how much weight
more manageable weight. Likewise, participants who
simulate 20 pounds may shift upward to a more realistic they thought they could lift. People were allowed 5 min to complete
weight. If this account is accurate, people who simulate a each survey. Following completion of the health survey, the exper-
imenter asked the participants in the 5- and 10-simulation condi-
larger amount of weight should always report that they can
tions whether they noticed the relationship between the two studies
lift more weight. To evaluatethis anchoringhypothesis,we
and to describe the exact nature of this relationship. The people in the
compared the weight estimates from a group who imag-
0
-simulation condition did not experience any simulation, and there-
ined lifting200 poundswith thosefrom a group who imag-
ined lifting 400 pounds. If people are anchoring their es-
fore these participants only completed the health survey.
The participants in Experiment 2B listened to the mental simula-
timates, people in the 400-pounds condition should start tion exercise two times, completed the imagery survey, and then
with a higher value before adjusting to a more realistic completed the health survey. The participants in the 20-pounds con-
dition mentally simulated lifting 20 pounds, whereas the participants
weight estimate. Because they start with a higher value, it
in the 200-pounds condition simulated lifting 200 pounds. Other than
is likely that the adjustment process will produce a higher
the change in weight, these two conditions were identical. The par-
ticipants in the control condition did not perform a mental simula-
weight estimate. Therefore, if anchoringis a viable expla-
nation, people in the 400-pounds condition should pro-
vide a higher weight estimate than do people in the 200-
pounds condition.
tion exercise and, therefore, only filled out the health survey.
In Experiment 2C, we used the same procedures and materials as
those used in Experiment 2B, except that the participants in the 200-
pounds condition mentally simulated lifting 200 pounds, whereas the
participants in the 400-pounds condition simulated lifting 400 pounds.
As with the other control conditions, these participants did not
imagine lifting any weight.
Method
Participants. In Experiment 2A, 82 undergraduates were ran-
domly assigned to perform 0 simulations (n 5 27), 5 simulations
(
n 5 22), or 10 simulations (n 5 33). In Experiment 2B, 129 under-
graduates were assigned to the control (n 5 44), 20-pounds (n 5
Results and Discussion
3
4
5), or 200-pounds (n 5 40) condition. In Experiment 2C, 119 un-
dergraduates were randomly assigned to the 200-pound (n 5 36),
00-pounds (n 5 36), or control (n 5 47) condition. In all three ex-
periments, the participants were tested in small groups of 8–10, and between the imagery and the health survey studies. It is
Experiment 2A. None of the participantsin the 5- and
1
0-simulation conditionsreported detectinga relationship
4
none of them had taken part in Experiment 1.
therefore unlikely that these people intentionally inflated
their weight estimates on the health survey. The partici-
pants in the 0-simulations condition estimated that they
could lift 88 pounds (SE 5 9.09), the participantsin the 5-
simulationsconditionestimated111 pounds(SE 5 17.16),
and the participants in the 10-simulation condition aver-
Materials and Procedure. At the outset of the experimental ses-
sion, the participants in the simulation conditions were informed that
they would take part in two separate studies. The first study was an
investigation of how people mentally simulate an event, and the sec-
ond study consisted of a health survey. In order to increase the ef-
fectiveness of the cover story, one experimenter conducted the men-
tal simulation study, and a second experimenter administered the aged 132 pounds(SE 5 13.4). The overallanalysisof vari-
health survey.
In Experiment 2A, the participants in the 5- and 10-simulation
conditions listened to the following minute-long audiotaped in-
structions:
ance (ANOVA) fell just shy of conventionalstatisticalsig-
nificance [F(2,79) 5 2.8, p 5 .06]. However, a significant
linear trend was present [F(1,79) 5 5.7, p , .02]. Those
participantswho completed more mental simulation trials
believed that they could lift more weight than did the par-
ticipants who completed fewer simulations. These results
fit nicely with the misattribution account. In this case, in-
creasing the number of simulation trials causes the details
of the simulation to come to mind more easily, which in
turn causes people to inflate their weight estimates. These
results are consistentwith Goff and Roediger (1998), who
reported that the number of memory distortionsincreased
as the number of imaginingsincreased.
Close your eyes, feel confident that you will be able to lift the 200 pound
barbells, mentally picture yourself lying on the gray weight bench with
the bar above your head, concentrate on lifting the bar while blocking out
other sights and sounds, take a look at the two 100-pound weight plates,
grab the bar, take a deep breath and gently lift it off the stand, gradually
thrust it in the air and breathe out, return the bar to the stand and get up
from the bench.
The mental simulation instructions were repeated 5 or 10 times,
with a short rest interval between each repetition. The participants then
completed the 12-item survey that assessed the qualitative aspects
of their simulation experience and further supported the notion that
this was an investigation of their mental simulation experience. For
At least two additionalaspects of these data are partic-
instance, people were asked the following questions. What color were ularly noteworthy. First, if demand characteristics were a